Docsity
Docsity

Prepare for your exams
Prepare for your exams

Study with the several resources on Docsity


Earn points to download
Earn points to download

Earn points by helping other students or get them with a premium plan


Guidelines and tips
Guidelines and tips

Predicting the Violent Offender: The Discriminant Validity of the Subculture of Violence, Papers of Criminology

Written by 4 criminal justice professors JEAN MARIE MCGLOIN, CHRISTOPHER J. SCHRECK, ERIC A. STEWART, GRAHAM C. OUSEY.

Typology: Papers

2021/2022

Uploaded on 03/31/2022

nicoth
nicoth 🇺🇸

4.3

(20)

262 documents

1 / 28

Toggle sidebar

This page cannot be seen from the preview

Don't miss anything!

bg1
PREDICTING THE VIOLENT OFFENDER:
THE DISCRIMINANT VALIDITY OF THE
SUBCULTURE OF VIOLENCE
JEAN MARIE MCGLOIN
Department of Criminology and Criminal Justice
University of MarylandCollege Park
CHRISTOPHER J. SCHRECK
Department of Criminal Justice
Rochester Institute of Technology
ERIC A. STEWART
College of Criminology and Criminal Justice
Florida State University
GRAHAM C. OUSEY
Department of Sociology
College of William and Mary
KEYWORDS: violence, specialization, subculture of violence
This study tests the extent to which an adherence to the subculture
of violence uniquely predicts a tendency to favor violence or instead
predicts a more generalized offending re pertoire, of which violence is
part. Specifically, we use a unique analytic technique that pr ovides the
opportunity to distinguish empirically between the “violent offender”
and/or the “frequent offender.” The results suggest that holding values
favorable toward violence consistently predicts general offending but do
not identify youth who systematically favor violence over nonviolence.
This discussion considers the impact of these findings for the continued
utility of the subculture of violence perspective.
The authors wish to thank Raymond Paternoster and D. Wayne Osgood for
their helpful reviews of earlier versions of this article. Direct correspondence
to Jean Marie McGloin, Department of Criminology and Criminal Justice, Uni-
versity of Maryland, 2220 LeFrak Hall, College Park, MD 20742 (e-mail:
jmcgloin@umd.edu).
C 2 011 American Society of Criminology doi: 10.1111/j.1745-9125.2011.00235.x
CRIMINOLOGY Volume 49 Number 3 2011 767
pf3
pf4
pf5
pf8
pf9
pfa
pfd
pfe
pff
pf12
pf13
pf14
pf15
pf16
pf17
pf18
pf19
pf1a
pf1b
pf1c

Partial preview of the text

Download Predicting the Violent Offender: The Discriminant Validity of the Subculture of Violence and more Papers Criminology in PDF only on Docsity!

PREDICTING THE VIOLENT OFFENDER:

THE DISCRIMINANT VALIDITY OF THE

SUBCULTURE OF VIOLENCE

JEAN MARIE MCGLOIN

Department of Criminology and Criminal Justice

University of Maryland—College Park

CHRISTOPHER J. SCHRECK

Department of Criminal Justice

Rochester Institute of Technology

ERIC A. STEWART

College of Criminology and Criminal Justice

Florida State University

GRAHAM C. OUSEY

Department of Sociology

College of William and Mary

KEYWORDS: violence, specialization, subculture of violence

This study tests the extent to which an adherence to the subculture

of violence uniquely predicts a tendency to favor violence or instead

predicts a more generalized offending repertoire, of which violence is

part. Specifically, we use a unique analytic technique that provides the

opportunity to distinguish empirically between the “violent offender”

and/or the “frequent offender.” The results suggest that holding values

favorable toward violence consistently predicts general offending but do

not identify youth who systematically favor violence over nonviolence.

This discussion considers the impact of these findings for the continued

utility of the subculture of violence perspective.

∗ The authors wish to thank Raymond Paternoster and D. Wayne Osgood for

their helpful reviews of earlier versions of this article. Direct correspondence

to Jean Marie McGloin, Department of Criminology and Criminal Justice, Uni-

versity of Maryland, 2220 LeFrak Hall, College Park, MD 20742 (e-mail:

jmcgloin@umd.edu).

C 2011 American Society of Criminology doi: 10.1111/j.1745-9125.2011.00235.x

CRIMINOLOGY Volume 49 Number 3 2011 767

768 MCGLOIN ET AL.

Given the ample empirical attention directed at identifying what pro-

duces a violent offender, one might reasonably assume that criminologists

consider it a worthy area of inquiry. Indeed, the fact that such literature

guides the development and structure of intervention programs aimed at

curbing youth violence underscores this point (e.g., Reiss and Roth, 1993).

Yet there is a fundamental debate in criminology about the “uniqueness”

of the violent offender and, consequently, whether he deserves research

attention. Many leading criminological theorists are skeptical of the claim

that offenders specialize in any meaningful way, thus questioning the need

for theory to classify offenders based on the type of crimes they commit

or for policies to address supposedly unique proclivities (Felson, 1994;

Gottfredson and Hirschi, 1990; Hirschi and Gottfredson, 2008; Sampson

and Laub, 1993). Others disagree, however, arguing that some factors exist

that have special relevance in the etiology of violence, which are worthy

of attention and inquiry (e.g., Anderson, 1999; Wolfgang and Ferracuti,

1967). In the end, this debate is no small matter; at issue is the question

about what sort of criminological theory—general or specifc/typological—

the feld should pursue and value.

Even though the empirical literature has identifed a large stack of in-

dividual and social factors as purported predictors of violent offending, it

does little to clarify this debate. Most of these prior analyses have focused

on predicting the rate or frequency of violence, not on the portion of

the total offenses that are violent (e.g., Elliott, 1994; Piquero et al., 2005;

Stewart and Simons, 2006). This approach is effective insofar as it allows

researchers to identify predictors of violence, but it does so at the price of

being unable to establish which factors uniquely predict violence. Because

frequent offenders engage in violence, as well as exhibit many other forms

of antisocial behavior, studies of violence typically are not identifying a

distinct violent offender so much as predicting “violent offenses [that] are

essentially committed at random in prolifc criminal careers” (Farrington,

1998: 429; see also MacDonald, Haviland, and Morral, 2009).

This limitation is especially salient when considering theories that are

explicitly meant to identify violent offenders. Specifcally, determining

whether some offenders systematically favor violence and identifying fac-

tors that distinguish such people from others is of paramount importance

for assessing the validity of the subculture of violence perspective. This

framework asserts that the adherence to violent norms and beliefs should

identify individuals prone toward violence, not simply those who spo-

radically engage in violence as part of a versatile offending profle. Al-

though numerous studies have observed a relationship between violent at-

titudes/values and aggressive behavior (e.g., Baron, 2001; Hartnagel, 1980;

Heimer, 1997; Kubrin and Weitzer, 2003; Markowitz and Felson, 1998;

Stewart and Simons, 2006, 2010), they have focused only on violence as

770 MCGLOIN ET AL.

sought to explain a particular sort of deviant behavior: violence. Although

the people to whom the subculture of violence applies has varied—from

Southerners to African Americans and even to Scotch Irish clans settled

in Appalachian communities (Anderson, 1990; Dixon and Lizotte, 1987;

Fischer, 1989; Hackney, 1969; Messner, 1983; Nisbett and Cohen, 1996)—

the unifying core concept remains the same. Individuals who adhere to a

normative system that tolerates and endorses violence as a way of managing

social interactions and disputes have a systematic heightened proclivity for

being aggressive (Felson et al., 1994; Ousey and Wilcox,2005).

This perspective gained traction largely because of Wolfgang and Ferra-

cutti’s (1967) research on homicide rates in Philadelphia. Based on obser-

vations of differential homicide patterns, they argued that for some groups

of people in disadvantaged settings, violence is a frequent and normative

way of managing most dimensions of social life, from parenting, to romantic

relationships, to basic social exchanges. From the beginning, then, it is im-

portant to recognize that this theoretical premise was developed to explain

violence, but because Wolfgang and Ferracutti (1967) looked specifcally

at homicide trends, it was empirically unclear whether this subculture

discriminated violence from other forms of crime and deviance. Even so,

this perspective endured and several scholars offered their own versions

of the subculture of violence (see Bernard, 1990; Luckenbill and Doyle,

1989). Most recently, Anderson (1990, 1999) argued that an oppositional

subculture oriented around violence and aggression has developed in highly

disadvantaged neighborhoods, particularly among a segment of African

American residents.

The key goal among those who adhere to the subculture of violence

is the development and maintenance of a tough reputation, which when

compared with traditional middle-class markers of success such as edu-

cation and good employment, is relatively ephemeral, easily damaged, or

“stolen.” For instance, Anderson (1990) noted that Black youth in highly

disadvantaged circumstances can have their source of respect and success

snatched away in a momentary interaction, thereby forcing them to develop

a willingness to ward off perceived threats and reinforce this reputation. In

this way, for those who adhere to violent norms, mundane interactions that

are trivial at the outset can easily escalate into serious violence, given the

normative status aggression has with regard to interpreting and responding

to social situations. Several scholars have spoken about this subculture in

terms of the cognitive scripts that individuals import into situations and rely

on when interpreting and responding to social interactions (Luckenbill and

Doyle, 1989; Wilkinson and Fagan, 1996; see also Lee and Ousey, 2011).

Whereas someone who does not adhere to the subculture of violence may

view being bumped by a passerby as accidental or perhaps somewhat rude,

individuals who adhere to the subculture of violence are likely to interpret

PREDICTINGTHEVIOLENT OFFENDER 771

this bump as a sign of disrespect that demands an aggressive response.^2

Thus, whereas subculture of delinquency theories seek to identify individ-

uals who are more likely to engage in deviance than their counterparts,

the subculture of violence framework is invested in explaining why some

individuals are more likely to appeal to violence.

As stated, most prior studies testing the validity of the subculture of

violence have focused on explaining variation in measures of the overall

level of violence, not the extent to which violence is more or less prevalent

in relation to other types of offending behavior. For instance, Stewart,

Simons, and Conger (2002: 813) studied the neighborhood and psycholog-

ical predictors of violence among a sample of African American youth.

They measured whether subjects subscribed to a street code according to

the level of agreement with statements, such as “people will take advantage

of you if you don’t let them know how tough you are” and “people tend

to respect a person who is tough and aggressive.” Their results demon-

strated a positive relationship between adherence to the street code and

violent behavior (see also Stewart and Simons, 2006). Because no other

outcomes were considered, however, it remains unclear whether the street

code primarily promotes violence or whether it simply leads to a higher

overall rate of general offending, of which violence is one component.

This same issue applies to the majority of empirical work testing the

subculture of violence (e.g., Baron, Kennedy, and Forde, 2001; Bernburg

and Thorlindsson, 2007; Markowitz and Felson, 1998; Stewart and Simons,

2010).

Perhaps the closest acknowledgment of this problem is found in the

work of Felson et al. (1994), who examined whether the subculture of

violence predicted violent as well as nonviolent crime (theft and vandalism).

They found that an adherence to the subculture of violence increased

the likelihood of engaging in violence, as well as in nonviolent criminal

behaviors, which argues against this perspective as a “specialist” theory of

violent offending (see also Bernburg and Thorlindsson, 1999). They stated:

“[V]alues regarding violence generally predict other forms of delinquency

as well as they predict violence. Previous research on the subculture of

violence does not concern itself with this issue since it never examines other

forms of delinquent behavior” (Felson et al., 1994: 168). The question then

emerges, is the fact that the subculture of violence predicts nonviolent crime

2. Felson et al. (1994) have observed that there are two layers to the subculture of

violence. The frst is the normative system that defnes the subculture. The second

is social control, whereby if the rules of the street are not followed as expected,

this can result in sanctions for one’s reputation and consequently physical safety

(see also Stewart, Schreck, and Simons, 2006).

PREDICTINGTHEVIOLENT OFFENDER 773

DATA AND METHODS

DATA

Data for this study were drawn from the Rural Substance Abuse and

Violence Project (RSVP), a prospective four-wave panel study of adoles-

cents residing in the state of Kentucky (NIDA Grant DA-11317) during

the period between 2001 and 2004. The RSVP project data were collected

initially in the spring of 2001 when the sample was in the seventh grade,

and subsequent measurements took place during the spring of the next

three years. The RSVP sample involved a multistage procedure with a ran-

dom selection of 30 (of 120) counties drawn from population-based strata.

Within the 30 selected counties, each of the 74 public schools with seventh

graders was contacted and access was granted by 65 schools. The targeted

population was 9,488 seventh graders enrolled in those 65 middle schools

at the initiation of the study. Active parent consent was required because

underage study participants were identifed and followed across multiple

years. Using a “Dillman method” for mailed surveys (Dillman, 1978), ac-

tive consent was granted by 43 percent of parents, leaving 4,102 sample

participants with parental approval. Using mass administration methods

at the participating schools, completed surveys were obtained from 3,

students in wave I and 3,638 students in wave II. In waves III and IV, after

most students had traversed from the originally sampled middle schools

into their local high school, completed surveys were obtained from 3,

and 3,040 respondents, respectively. Across the 4 years of study, at least

one observation was recorded for 3,976 of the original 4,102 respondents

whose parents granted active consent.^4

DEPENDENT VARIABLES

Our outcome measures are based on four self-report items measuring ap-

proximately how frequently within the current school year the respondent

physically attacked someone else or had stolen someone else’s money or

property, with separate items for offending at school and away from school.

Responses are coded using an ordinal metric: 1 = never, 2 = less than once

a month, 3 = approximately once a month, 4 = approximately 1–2 times

per week, and 5 = daily or almost daily.^5 As is typical with offense data,

4. For additional details on sample characteristics and attrition, see Ousey and

Wilcox (2007).

5. Poisson regression assumes that the lowest score is zero, so for the multivariate

analyses (which will be described in more detail later), we recoded these items

by subtracting one from the original score. Descriptive statistics, such as those

reported in the narrative and in table 3, employ the original coding.

774 MCGLOIN ET AL.

the responses are positively skewed, with most respondents reporting they

had not engaged in any of the offense activities (the mean scores for each

item in all four waves are reported in appendix A). Confrmatory factor

analyses show that the offense items load well onto the primary factor, thus

showing that they are statistically homogeneous; however, the second factor

loading also shows some evidence of distinctiveness between the violence

and nonviolence items. That is, even though all offense measures are pos-

itively correlated with each other, the violence items are somewhat more

strongly correlated with other violence items than they are with nonviolent

items, with a similar pattern unfolding for the nonviolent items. As Osgood

and Schreck noted (2007: 287–8), this pattern is consistent with the basic

assumptions of their approach, where the data reveal latent variables for

both overall offending and specialization in violence.

INDEPENDENT VARIABLE

Subculture of Violence

The Kentucky data contained four items that are suitable for our pur-

poses (see also Ousey and Wilcox, 2005). These items ask respondents

their level of agreement with the following: “Beating up other kids to gain

respect,” “beating up others who call you a dirty name,” “beating up others

who start a fght with you,” and “hitting other people is acceptable to get

what one wants.” Responses were coded as 1 (^) =strongly disagreethrough

(^4) =strongly agree. Respondents indicated that, on average, they disagreed

with these value statements, with mean scores hovering around 1.70 for

all four waves. Alpha reliability tests indicate a generally acceptable level

of internal consistency for our street codes measure across waves ( α (^) =

mid- .70s for waves I through III and .64 for wave IV).^6 Table 1 contains

6. We also created measures of attitudes favoring nonviolent offending as a form

of sensitivity analysis in order to address the possibility that beliefs about crime,

whether violent or nonviolent, tended to correlate strongly with each other. In

other words, we wanted to ensure that our SOV measure was not instead mea-

suring attitudes toward deviance generally. This nonviolent index contained items

asking about how wrong it was to steal, vandalize, or break into buildings in order

to steal. This index was statistically distinct from the subculture of violence beliefs

measure (i.e., the violent belief items that comprise our SOV measure and the

nonviolent belief items loaded on two different factors). Furthermore, including it

in the regression models did not result in any changes with regard to the predictive

abilities of the subculture of violence measure (for either general offending or

specialization in violence). We elected not to report these sets of supplemental

results because a careful consideration of this measure, its theoretical relevance,

and the models that contain it requires more attention than we have space for

here. In the end, the key issue is that the SOV measure seems not to be measuring

beliefs about general deviance, instead capturing our concept of interest.

776 MCGLOIN ET AL.

Friends’ Specialization in Violence

Given the robust relationship between deviant peers’ attitudes and vari-

ous dimensions of offending behavior (Warr, 2002), we control for friends’

tendency to specialize in violence, which measures the contrast between

reported violent and nonviolent offending. The index comprised two items,

which ask how many friends 1) stole something and 2) physically attacked

someone. Having at least one friend who attacked someone increased the

friend’s violent specialization index score by 1, whereas having at least one

friend who engaged in theft reduced the index score by 1. The mean friends’

specialization scores were approximately+.15 across each of the four

waves. Although this index has obvious potential theoretical signifcance

(e.g., differential association theory), we only employ this item as a control,

given that there are legitimate concerns about the validity of respondents’

claims about their friend’s behavior (Gottfredson and Hirschi, 1990; Haynie

and Osgood, 2005).^8

Maternal Attachment

Maternal attachment is an item often linked with offending (e.g., Hirschi,

1969), although not necessarily specialization. Attachment to mother is

based on the average response by subjects to four items: mother under-

stands me, mother makes me feel wanted, I share my thoughts with mother,

and I talk to my mother. Scores range from 1 (never) through 5 (always),

with mean scores being fairly consistent across the waves (from 3.8 through

4.0, meaning that the children felt that these statements “often” character-

ized their relationships with their mother).

Demographic Measures

Our analysis also incorporates the standard demographic controls: so-

cioeconomic status, age, race (1 (^) = African American), and gender (1 =

male). We have two measures of socioeconomic status: parental education

and parental unemployment. For parents’ education, we selected the parent

with the higher level of education because many children only have one

parent. This ordinal variable ranges from 1 (grade school or less) to 7

(graduate or professional school). Parents’ unemployment reports whether

at least one parent was unemployed for a given wave (1 = yes and 0 = no).

8. In supplementary models, we omitted friends’ specialization in violence to see

whether this measure was unduly infuencing the results; the basic pattern reported

in the text changed only slightly, with just two of the seven models now revealing

a signifcant effect for subcultural beliefs about violence. This, in our view, is not

compelling evidence to change our conclusions with respect to subculture’s ability

to infuence specialization.

PREDICTINGTHEVIOLENT OFFENDER 777

During the 4 years of observation, these measures remained consistent. The

average respondent had at least one parent with some college education,

and parental unemployment ranged from 5 percent to 10 percent. Age is a

continuous measure, with the average respondent 13.4 years old at wave I.

Race was coded as 1 African American (0 all others), and gender was= =

coded as 1 male and 0 female. Approximately 48 percent of the sample= =

was male and 5 percent were African American. As with the socioeconomic

status (SES) measures, the demographic profles across each wave did not

meaningfully change.

ANALYTIC METHODS

Our statistical approach uses Osgood and Schreck’s (2007) item response

theory (IRT)-based statistical model for detecting specialization in offend-

ing. As this approach has been employed in several studies to date (e.g.,

Schreck, McGloin, and Kirk, 2009; Schreck, Stewart, and Osgood, 2008;

Sullivan et al., 2009), the presentation will only describe the main features

of the model.

The Osgood and Schreck model incorporates two levels of analysis,

where individual offense items are nested within individual subjects.^9 The

level 1 model works to defne two latent measures that are the focus of

the analysis: overall offending ( β (^) 0 j ) and offending specialization ( β (^) 1 j ). To

defne the specialization index, the model includes a group-mean-centered

dummy variable, “Spec,” which takes on positive scores for violent offense

items and negative scores for nonviolent offense items. The coeffcient as-

sociated with this specialization variable reports the difference in the log of

the expected event-rate for violent offense items to the log of the expected

9. In the notation of hierarchical linear modeling (Raudenbush and Bryk, 2002), our

level 1 regression equation is:

L I −^1

ln( λij ) = β 0 j + β 1 j Spec + βij D ij (1)

i = 2

The level 2 regression equations are:

β 0 j = γ 00 + γ 01 X 1 j + γ 02 X 2 j + ... + u 0 j (2)

β 1 j = γ 11 X 1 j + γ 12 X 2 j +. .. + u 1 j (3)

βij = γi 0 (4)

In equation 1, the intercept, β 0 j , refers to the average score for all offenses, β 1 j

is the specialization coeffcient, and the remainder incorporates the base rates for

the individual offense items through dummy variables indicative of each item.

PREDICTINGTHEVIOLENT OFFENDER 779

Table 2. Reliability and Variance of Overall Offending and

Specialization

Wave I Wave II Wave III Wave IV Overall Offending Reliability Variance ( τ ) Specialization Reliability

Variance ( τ ) 5_._ 89(. 38) 6_._ 46(. 42) 6_._ 92(. 54) 8_._ 05(. 78)

NOTE: Standard errors of τ in parentheses.

RESULTS

Our frst step is to determine whether specialization in violence exists to a degree that is statistically signifcant and is relatively stable over time (see also Osgood and Schreck, 2007; Sullivan et al., 2009). Table 2 reports the variances, standard errors, and reliability scores for the two latent variables defned by the level 1 measurement model: overall offending and specialization. Some prior research has found that reliability scores for overall offending are relatively high (i.e., in the .70–.80 range; see Osgood and Schreck, 2007); however, our scores for each of the four waves are com-

paratively low and are more consistent with those reported in Sullivan et al.

(2009). Previous research using the IRT method suggests that specialization

is relatively unreliably measured, owing to the relatively few members of

the sample who self-report much offense activity, and the reliabilities of

the specialization index in the RSVP data conform to this expectation. The

implication of this is that specialization becomes more diffcult to detect

and predict largely because of the limited information coming from most of

the sample (i.e., we are dealing with a lot of zeroes or ones in these data).

An approach that can take this into account, such as our latent variable

approach, can alleviate this problem while using data from everyone in the

sample. To assess the statistical signifcance of the degree of specialization

in the sample, we compute a z score by obtaining the ratio of the variance

component to its standard error. For all four waves, the ratio exceeds 10,

indicating that there is statistically signifcant differentiation in individual

offense patterns from the population base-rates (i.e., specialization), with

less than a .0001 probability that the observed pattern is a function of

chance.

Table 3 provides one illustration of the variation in offending special- ization, focusing on the most frequent offenders in the Kentucky data

this would exclude most subjects and the information they provide. For more, see

Osgood and Schreck (2007).

780 MCGLOIN ET AL.

Table 3. Observed Violent and Nonviolent Offending

Averages, by Level of Specialization

Observed Distribution of Offending

Specialization Violent Nonviolent (^) n Wave I Violent ( > 1 SD) 3.5 1.0 140 Neither ( > – 1 SD and < 1 SD) 3.3 2.3 120 Nonviolent ( < – 1 SD) 2.1 3.5 37 Wave II Violent ( > 1 SD) 3.3 1.0 145 Neither ( > – 1 SD and < 1 SD) 2.9 2.5 163 Nonviolent ( < – 1 SD) 1.3 3.2 16 Wave III Violent ( > 1 SD) 2.6 1.0 142 Neither ( > – 1 SD and < 1 SD) 2.6 2.3 123 Nonviolent ( < – 1 SD) 1.0 2.2 33 Wave IV Violent ( > 1 SD) 2.6 1.0 104 Neither ( > – 1 SD and < 1 SD) 2.5 2.4 79 Nonviolent ( < – 1 SD) 1.0 2.1 20

NOTE: 1 = never, 2 = less than once a month, 3 = 1–2 times per week, and 4 = almost daily.

(i.e., the 90th percentile of overall offending in our sample, as defned by

having the highest average score for the four offense items). This table clas-

sifes the high-frequency offenders into three categories, based on whether

their pattern of offending favors violence, nonviolence, or is fairly equitable

across offense types. The values in the “observed distribution of offending”

column report the average score on the two violent offending items and

the average score on the nonviolent offending items. Among those whose

specialization score was 1 standard deviation above the mean in wave I—

indicating a violent specialist—we fnd that he or she reported engaging

in each of the assault items between “once or twice a week” to “once a

month.” In contrast, the 1.0 average for the nonviolent offending items is

indicative of no nonviolent activity whatsoever (1 never). Moving to the=

next row, we see that those frequent offenders who fell within 1 standard

deviation of the mean in wave I exhibited some tendency to favor violence,

but the differential was not meaningfully large enough to be distinct. Such

individuals reported, on average, committing each of the offense items,

whether violent or nonviolent, up to about once a month. Turning to the

fnal row, we see that those offenders who were classifed as “nonviolent”

indicated rarely participating in violent offenses (averaging a 2.1 and a 1.

score on each violent offense for the frst two waves and 1.0 for the last two).

In contrast, they engaged in each nonviolent offense at least once a month

or more, except for waves III and IV where the averages declined to “less

than once a month.”

782

Table 5. Relationships of Explanatory Variables to Overall Offending and Specialization from

Poisson-Based Hierarchical Linear Model, Cross-Sectional Models

Wave I Wave II Wave III Wave IV

Overall Overall Overall Overall Crime Specialization Crime Specialization Crime Specialization Crime Specialization

Variables γ SE γ SE γ SE γ SE γ SE γ SE γ SE γ SE

Male_._ 62 ∗^ .13 −. 19 .20_._ 53 ∗^ .14 −. 29 .21_._ 67 ∗^ .16 −. 31 .23_._ 60 ∗^ .19_._ 32. Black_._ 45 .27_._ 10 .37_._ 65 ∗^ .27_._ 33 .42_._ 97 ∗^ .29 −. 38 .44_._ 97 ∗^ .40 −. 73. Age_._ 12 .11 −. 10 .17_._ 02 .12 −. 07 .19_._ 13 .15_._ 37 .21_._ 35 .18 −. 27. Parents’ education_._ 04 .04 −. 05 .06_._ 03 .04 −. 03 .06 −. 01 .05 −. 05 .06_._ 12 ∗^ .06 −. 04. Parents unemployed_._ (^06) .20 −. 56 .29_._ 49 .28 −. 72 .39_._ 57 .34_._ 27 .58_._ 11 .42 −. 55. Impulsivity_._ 71 ∗^ .09 −. 29 ∗^ .13_._ 78 ∗^ .10 −. 22 .14_._ 77 ∗^ .11 −. 17 .14_._ 72 ∗^ .13_._ 04. SOV_._ 99 ∗^ .09 −. 05 .13 1_._ 00 ∗^ .10_._ 19 .13 1_._ 10 ∗^ .12_._ 29 .15 1_._ 81 ∗^ .14 −. 18. Attachment to mother −. 45 ∗^ .06_._ 17 .09 −. 48 ∗^ .07_._ 05 .11 −. 34 ∗^ .09 −. 01 .11 −. 37 ∗^ .10 −. 02. Friends’ specialization_._ 12 .13 1_._ 68 ∗ .20_._ 11 .16 2_._ 71 ∗ .27 −. 20 .17 2_._ 19 ∗ .28_._ 21 .21 1_._ 86 ∗.

NOTE: γ is the hierarchical linear modeling population average estimate, and SE is its robust standard error. ABBREVIATIONS: SE (^) = standard error; SOV (^) =subculture of violence. ∗ p < .05.

MCGLOIN ET AL.

PREDICTINGTHEVIOLENT OFFENDER 783

Additionally, being male, having weak maternal attachment, and impulsiv-

ity are also related with higher levels of (general) illegal behavior.

The most important question, however, is whether the independent vari-

ables are associated with the tendency to favor violence over nonviolence.

As noted earlier in our description of the models, regression coeffcients

in the specialization equation indicate how each unit change in a predic-

tor variable affects the ratio of violent-to-nonviolent offenses. Our key

theoretical hypothesis concerned subcultural values, which we predicted

would be associated with a tendency to specialize in violent behavior. This

prediction was not supported in any of the waves. Children who reported

stronger beliefs in the use of violence were statistically neither more nor less

likely to engage in violence than other subjects, although they engaged in

more crime overall. The only measure to predict specialization tendencies

consistently was the tendency for friends to specialize, and this measure

did so in all four waves. This measure is somewhat risky to interpret,

however, because of the possibility that respondents could, at least partly,

be projecting their own qualities on their friends (see Haynie and Osgood,

2005). In short, although there is a signifcant tendency for many offenders

to specialize, our models do not consistently detect the reasons why, at least

among our key substantive predictors.^13

13. To add further rigor to our analyses, we examined longitudinal models with

lagged predictors (e.g., wave II offending and wave I predictors). In all cases, the

results reported in the cross-sectional models were confrmed. Subcultural beliefs

in violence still do not signifcantly infuence the probability that a person will

engage in a greater preponderance of violence relative to nonviolence. Such beliefs

do, however, correspond with higher levels of crime generally. These results are

available from the authors upon request. As an additional form of supplementary

analyses, we also ran models that predicted counts of violent crime and nonviolent

crime, which is more in line with previous analyses of the subculture of violence

(see appendix B, which presents one of the cross-sectional models, for wave I).

Under this specifcation (with the same control variables), subculture of violence

values predicted both violent and nonviolent offending. Had our analysis only

focused on violent offending, like the majority of previous analyses, we would have

most likely concluded that the subculture of violence explains violent behavior,

although we would not know if it uniquely predicted violence. Had we used both

models as our primary analysis, this would have left us in the same position as

Felson et al. (1994)—suspicious that the subculture of violence may instead be a

subculture of deviance, but unaware of whether it predicted a tendency to favor

violence over nonviolence (or vice versa). In contrast, our main models confrm

that adherence to the subculture of violence does not predict any preference for

violence over nonviolence, nor nonviolence over violence. Instead, it only predicts

(general) offending frequency.

PREDICTINGTHEVIOLENT OFFENDER 785

the same measure of subculture of violence beliefs does predict counts of violence (see appendix B). Had this been our plan of analysis, this study likely would have been added to the pile of those supportive of this theo- retical perspective. But, by relying on a method that distinguishes between general offending and violence specialization, it becomes clear that such models and fndings can be deceptive. Our fndings therefore stand in contrast to the core premise—if not the very name—of the subculture of violence. The primary reason why this theoretical perspective stood out from other subculture of deviance per- spectives was that it focused on identifying and describing the reasons for heightened violence among particular pockets of society. Yet, our results suggest that these supposed etiological factors and processes are instead identifying heightened levels offending, of which violence is a part.^14 To be clear, we are not joining other scholars in criticizing the subculture of violence perspective on the grounds that tendencies to specialize in violence do not exist (see Gottfredson and Hirschi, 1990)—they do. Rather, we are troubled by the fnding that measures refective of this perspective do not identify these tendencies (at least in the data used here). For decades, scholars have been skeptical of the empirical validity of the subculture of violence (e.g., Dixon and Lizotte, 1987; Erlanger, 1974; Felson et al., 1994; Gottfredson and Hirschi, 1990; Loftin and Hill, 1974), and the current study joins these voices. Given the now increasing evidence that at least some individuals “spe- cialize” in violent offending, the question now turns to what theories and constructs contained therein can predict a tendency to favor violent over nonviolent offending. Recent work confrming the presence of a latent tendency to favor violent offending largely has been atheoretical (e.g., Osgood and Schreck, 2007; Sullivan et al., 2009), and our analysis questions

a “usual suspect” as a consistent and dominant explanation of this special-

ization (i.e., the subculture of violence). Although not specifcally focused

on violence, other work on specialization has increasingly highlighted the

importance of opportunity structures in shaping crime profles, even if the

motivation for such behavior stems from an enduring propensity (McGloin

et al., 2007; McGloin, Sullivan, and Piquero, 2009). As work continues on this front, incorporating a situational perspective may turn out to be a fruitful direction of development (see also Wikstr o¨ m, 2006). Although we believe this study poses signifcant challenges to the pre- dictive validity of the subculture of violence premise, we recognize that

14. This raises the question of why violent attitudes would predict general offending.

One possibility rests on the idea that violence is usually regarded as more “serious”

than nonviolent offending. Individuals with attitudes that endorse violence may

therefore be willing to engage in both serious and minor crime.

786 MCGLOIN ET AL.

additional work is necessary to overcome the limitations of this piece. First,

many respondents in our data are youth drawn from a rural setting. Many

subculture of violence perspectives were developed to explain behavior

among residents in disadvantaged urban communities (although some also

were applied to rural communities; see Ayers, 1991; Nisbett and Cohen,

1996). Although our data certainly cover some signifcantly disadvantaged

geographic areas, it would be benefcial for future work to replicate our

analysis with data sets that capture youth who reside in more urban environ-

ments. Second, our data covered 4 years, but the sample was young (12–

years old on average across the years). Given that we know crime patterns

shift with age, as can the factors and mechanisms implicated in producing

offending, replicating this analysis with samples that extend further into

adulthood would provide an important assessment of the robustness of the

current fndings. Finally, we would be remiss to ignore that our measures

of offending are based on self-reports. Although scholars often favor self-

reports over offcial records of crime when studying offending behavior, it

is possible that youth were reticent to report whether they had attacked

another person, perhaps more so than whether they had stolen another’s

property. Of course, social desirability also may have led them to overreport

violence as a means of “looking tough.” In the end, all self-reports are

limited by such concerns as recall errors, lying, and telescoping—our data

are no different. Therefore, it would be ideal to confrm the results here

with alternative outcome measures.

For substantive reasons, this research was focused on the contrast

between assaults and thefts. Other theories make different distinctions,

such as between white-collar crime and other crimes (e.g., Friedrichs and

Schwartz, 2008), and to consider these claims was beyond the scope of

this article. Nevertheless, given appropriate data, these questions are just

as important and Osgood and Schreck’s (2007) approach can help assess

the validity of these theoretical claims. Future research could therefore

extend both measures used here to capture violence and nonviolence as

well as to clarify the extent to which some individuals favor other crime

“types” and whether such tendencies coincide with theoretical predictions

or established risk factors.

REFERENCES

Anderson, Elijah. 1990. StreetWise: Race, Class, and Change in an Urban

Community. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Anderson, Elijah. 1999. Code of the Street: Decency, Violence, and the Moral

Life of the Inner City. New York: Norton.